Wenn das Kind nicht das gewünschte Geschlecht hat – Gender Enttäuschung

Arne berichtet über Gender Disappointment, also die Enttäuschung darüber, dass ein neugeborenes Kind nicht das Geschlecht hat, welches man hoffte:

Wenn das Geschlecht des Babys nicht erfüllt, was Eltern sich wünschen, nennt sich das „Gender Disappointment“, Geschlechtsenttäuschung. Offizielle Studien zu diesem Thema gibt es kaum, in einem Artikel der „Cambridge University Press“ wird die Frage aufgeworfen, ob „Gender Disappointment“ ein Mental-Health- oder ein soziokulturelles Problem ist. Beantwortet wird die Frage nicht final, es wird lediglich darauf aufmerksam gemacht, dass es weiterhin ein Tabuthema ist. (…) Auf Tiktok finden sich unter dem Hashtag #genderdisappointment zahlreiche Videos, in denen vor allem Frauen über ihre Erfahrungen sprechen.

So auch Nadine (43): „Als ich endlich schwanger wurde, war ich fest davon überzeugt, dass es ein Mädchen wird. Ich hatte auch schon seit Jahren einen Namen. Doch als beim Ultraschall verkündet wurde, dass es ein Junge wird, brach eine Welt für mich zusammen. Ich hoffte bis zur Geburt noch auf ein Mädchen.“ Das habe sich dann geändert: „Heute liebe ich meinen Sohn über alles und kann mir gar nicht mehr vorstellen, eine Tochter zu haben – ich bin eine echte Jungs-Mama.“

Bei der 33-jährigen Samantha ging es über die Enttäuschung hinaus, wie sie erzählt: „Ich habe jegliche Freude über die Schwangerschaft verloren. Ich war richtig wütend auf das Ungeborene. Mein Umfeld war sehr verständnisvoll, obwohl ich wirklich gemeine Dinge über das Baby sagte. Heute liebe ich meinen kleinen Buben über alles.“

Ich kann mir schon vorstellen, dass einige Eltern gerade ein Kind des gleichen Geschlechts haben wollen, also Mütter eine Tochter und Väter einen Sohn, einfach weil man meint, dass man da mehr geben kann und mehr auf gleicher Wellenlänge liegt.

Dass sich das so hochschaukelt, dass man wirklich enttäuscht ist finde ich hingegen schon etwas unverständlich. Hauptsache gesund war eher mein Motto.

Wie man auch an den obigen Beispielen sieht spielt das Geschlecht letztendlich auch keine Rolle – Kinder an sich sind eben eigene Personen und haben als solche immer etwas wunderbares wenn man sie beim Aufwachsen betreut. Gut, ich habe das Glück, dass wir einen jungen und ein Mädchen haben, insofern beide Seiten kennenlernen und ich kann schon verstehen, wenn etwa jemand der zwei Töchter hat meint, dass jetzt ein Sohn ganz schön wäre.

Aber zwei Töchter hätten auch ihre Vorteile gehabt, zB weil sie vielleicht länger miteinander spielen können und gleichere Interessen haben. Das fällt bei meinen noch nicht so auf, weil in dem Alter beide noch Dinosaurier etc gut finden aber mal sehen wie sich das entwickelt.

„Weibliche Jäger jagen manchmal, doch die geschlechtsspezifische Arbeitsteilung ist real“

Kürzlich ging eine Studie durch die sozialen Medien, die anführte, dass die Einteilung „Männer= Jäger und Frauen = Sammler“ so nicht stimmt.

The sexual division of labor among human foraging populations has typically been recognized as involving males as hunters and females as gatherers. Recent archeological research has questioned this paradigm with evidence that females hunted (and went to war) throughout the Homo sapiens lineage, though many of these authors assert the pattern of women hunting may only have occurred in the past. The current project gleans data from across the ethnographic literature to investigate the prevalence of women hunting in foraging societies in more recent times. Evidence from the past one hundred years supports archaeological finds from the Holocene that women from a broad range of cultures intentionally hunt for subsistence. These results aim to shift the male-hunter female-gatherer paradigm to account for the significant role females have in hunting, thus dramatically shifting stereotypes of labor, as well as mobility.

Quelle: The Myth of Man the Hunter: Women’s contribution to the hunt across ethnographic contexts

Zu dem Thema an sich hatte ich hier schon einmal einen Artikel, in dem ich eine Studie dazu besprochen hatte, aus der auch diese Grafik ist:

Männer Frauen Jäger Jägerinnen

Wie man dort gut sieht sind viele Jagd-Aktivitäten eher männlich dominiert, aber haben gerade bei kleineren Wild eben auch einen gewissen Frauenanteil.

Fishing ist bei 86,7%, Gathering small land fauna ist bei 54,5% (wenn man das als Jagd verstehen will, weil da Tiere involviert sind), Gathering small quating Fauna gar bei 31,1%. Aber auch bei großen Landtieren sind es „nur“ 99,3% Männer.

Aus der oben angegebenen Studie zur Methodik:

Data used for this study included reports on what, when, and how hunting occurred in the cultural group. Ethnographic reports needed to include explicit information, in the form of tables or sentences that females went on hunting trips, and were involved in tracking, locating animals, and helping with the killing if applicable. Given that there is a difference between the phrase ‘women went hunting’ and ‘women accompanied the hunters’ it should be noted that we were looking for phrases along the lines of ‘women were hunting’ or ‘women killed animals,’ not references to the idea that women might be accompanying men “only” to carry the kills home, though obviously this does happen as well (e.g. [19]). Specific contributions such how much killing took place, and total calories from female-only kills were not written about frequently enough to warrant their assessment here.

If women were hunting, it was further investigated to see if the hunting was done purposely, whether women would go out with the intention to hunt, or whether women were hunting spontaneously (i.e. “opportunistically”); this might occur when women may have been doing a different task but if the opportunity arose, they would kill an animal. This was determined by explicit statements in the published literature or by a judgment based on the descriptions. Women’s involvement in hunting was determined by written documentation explicitly stating that women were hunting in that particular foraging society or were excluded and in some instances even forbidden to hunt. The most important subsistence activity was also compared to the relative frequency of women hunting. Additionally, the type of the game hunted was assembled into three categories of small, medium, and large. The type of game was defined by the relative size of the prey, hunting toolkit that was used, or if size was explicitly stated in the literature. For example, when looking at the Tiwi society of Australia, a study reported that Tiwi women regularly hunted small animals while the hunting of large game was a man’s activity, suggesting that women were involved in hunting small game only [20]. In instances where the type of game was not explicitly stated, it was determined from other clues in the report. For example, accounts of the Matses from the Amazon state that the women would strike their prey with large sticks and machetes, which would account for large game whereas other societies had documentation of small digging sticks or the killing of rodents, suggesting the prevalence of small game hunting [21]. The prevalence of women hunting with children and dogs was also recorded and analyzed based on statements in the literature.

Compiled data were analyzed to determine the frequency of females hunting, the type of hunting accomplished, as well as the relative size of game.

In der ebenfalls unter dem Link verlinkten Exel-Tabelle sieht man, dass nur nach „Dokumentierten Fällen für Sammeln/Jagen/Fischen durch Frauen“ und nicht nach Anteilen gesucht wurde und es wurde noch unterschieden zwischen „klein, mittel, groß, alle“ Tiere.

Kaum war die Studie veröffentlicht lief sie gerade durch diverse soziale Kanäle und fand relativ viel Beachtung.

Eine Erwiderung dazu greift Probleme der Studie auf:

Gendered divisions of labor are a feature of every known contemporary hunter-gatherer (forager) society. While gender roles are certainly flexible, and prominent and well-studied cases of female hunting do exist, it is more often men who hunt. A new study (Anderson et al., 2023) surveyed ethnographically known foragers and found that women hunt in 79% of foraging societies, with big-game hunting occurring in 33%. Based on this single type of labor, which is one among dozens performed in foraging societies, the authors question the existence of gendered division of labor altogether. As a diverse group of hunter-gatherer experts, we find that claims that foraging societies lack or have weak gendered divisions of labor are contradicted by empirical evidence. We conducted an in-depth examination of the data and methods of Anderson et al. (2023), finding evidence of sample selection bias and numerous coding errors undermining the paper’s conclusions. Anderson et al. (2023) have started a useful dialogue to ameliorate the potential misconception that women never hunt. However, their analysis does not contradict the wide body of empirical evidence for gendered divisions of labor in foraging societies. Furthermore, a myopic focus on hunting diminishes the value of contributions that take different forms and downplays the trade-offs foragers of both sexes routinely face. We caution against ethnographic revisionism that projects Westernized conceptions of labor and its value onto foraging societies.

Quelle: Female foragers sometimes hunt, yet gendered divisions of labor are real: a comment on Anderson et al. (2023) The Myth of Man the Hunter

Aus der Erwiderung zum Forschungsstand:

One of the most consistent and well-established empirical regularities in the study of contemporary hunter-gatherers (foragers) is the existence of a flexible sexual/gendered division of labor, within which men and women make different but complementary subsistence contributions. Men tend to spend much of their time hunting medium-to-large-sized game, but often return empty-handed, whereas women spend much of their time caring for young children, gathering plant foods, and sometimes hunting small game, typically more reliable food sources (R. B. Bliege Bird and Codding, 2015; Kelly, 2013; Marlowe, 2005, Marlowe, 2007; Murdock and Provost, 1973). Despite these complementary divisions of labor, which vary as a function of ecological factor (Marlowe, 2007), gender roles are not rigidly deterministic and vary with cultural context (Brightman, 1996; Noss and Hewlett, 2001). For instance, women make capable hunters (Goodman et al., 1985; Noss and Hewlett, 2001), with reports of female participation in hunting going back centuries (Hoffman, Farquharson, & Venkataraman, 2024), and men regularly gather and are involved in childcare (K. Endicott, 1974; Kramer, 2018; Marlowe, 2007; Turnbull, 1965; Wood and Marlowe, 2013). Among most foraging populations, however, women hunt rarely, and sometimes not at all. One estimate placed the frequency of women’s hunting across foraging societies at roughly 7% (Gurven and Hill, 2009).

Natürlich ist dort nichts absolut und jeder wird alle Möglichkeiten genutzt haben um Nahrung zu besorgen, sei es Sammeln oder Jagen.

Wir sind evtl schlicht wieder hier:

Hier kommen aber natürlich viele Faktoren zusammen: Körperkraft, Gefährlichkeit der Jagd, der Umstand, dass kleine Kinder und Jagd sich nicht gut vereinbaren lassen etc.

All das kann dazu führen, dass die Trennung doch wieder recht deutlich wird, aber aufgeweicht wird je nach dem, was die Situation gerade erfordert. Eine Herde interessanter Tiere zum erjagen kommt in die Gegend? Vielleicht wird der ganze Stamm aktiviert, evtl mit Positionen wie Treiber etc. Oder es ist nichts in der Nähe, also wird allgemein gesammelt. Oder es tauchen ein paar Mammuts auf: Nur die Männer machen sich auf. Oder es sind Feinde/Säbelzahntiger in der Nähe: Die Frauen verstecken sich, die Männer gehen jagen.

Aber weiter aus der Studie:

their claims about contemporary foraging societies are contradicted by the large existing literature on female hunting and the gendered division of labor. Misrepresentation of the ethnographic record devalues the many essential contributions of forager women (and men) beyond hunting and dispenses with a century of hard-won empirical research. In this piece, we critique the claims of Anderson et al. (2023), showing that multiple methodological failures all bias their results in the same direction.

Anderson et al. (2023) coded their data at the society level. They provide no evidence that they conducted a paragraph-level analysis, which has been standard in cross-cultural research for some years (Ember, 2007). It was not possible to ascertain where the relevant text was located that was used to determine women’s hunting. As a result, their analysis is not replicable.

So wie ich das verstehe haben sie Schilderungen über Völker ausgewertet ohne das sie diese auf der  individuellen Ebene hinterfragt habe. Das macht die Studie anfällig dafür, dass hier – wie ja in der Ethnologie durchaus häufig – das, was der Ethnologe gerne als Gesellschaft gehabt hätte, dargestellt wird.

Further, their coding scheme does not account for the frequency of women’s hunting in a society in terms of effort allocated or the amount of prey acquired. Women’s hunting was coded as a binary variable and recorded as ‘present’ whether the case was a single report or habitual involvement. There was no minimum threshold for inclusion and, e.g., societies where hunting constituted only 1.2% of women’s returns (Marlowe., 2010) were coded as having women’s hunting. Beyond brief commentary on ‘opportunism’ in hunting, the results of Anderson et al. (2023) give little indication about the importance or broader societal context of women’s hunting activities.

Das war mir auch schon aufgefallen, es ist eben nur eine binäre Auswertung, die es vollkommen offen lässt, ob „Frauen“ (als Gruppe) jagen oder „Eine Frau“.

Etwas anderes ausgedrückt: Sie haben hier letztendlich einen Strohmann aufgebaut („Männer sind immer Jäger, Frauen immer nur Sammler“) und ihn dann durch auf diesem Strohmann aufbauenden Daten widerlegt.

Anderson et al. (2023) found evidence for women’s hunting in 50/63 (79%) of forager societies surveyed. For this estimate to be representative of actual contemporary forager diversity, the sampling procedure would need to be unbiased. Anderson et al. (2023) describe their sampling procedure as follows: they chose 391 forager societies from the D-PLACE (https://d-place.org/) ethnographic database (Kirby et al., 2016) and selected that subset of 63 in which ‘explicit’ information about hunting was available. To account for autocorrelation (Galton’s problem), societies were chosen in geographically diverse locations. Little further information about sampling was provided in the text of the article. However, the senior author told the newspaper El Pais — for an article entitled ‘Women have always hunted as much as men’ — that societies were only included where sources were explicitly “detailing hunting behavior and strategies”. Studies lacking tables, statistics or details, were excluded from the sample (Criado, 2023). The methods did not incorporate other statistical procedures that are common in quantitative ethnographic analysis, and the inclusion criteria were neither well described nor clearly operationalized. Given that the resulting estimate of 79% provided by Anderson et al. (2023) is substantially higher than previous quantitative assessments of 7% (Gurven and Hill, 2009), it is worthwhile to consider selection bias.

Man darf annehmen, dass die Kulturen, in denen etwas ungewöhnlicher verläuft, also evtl Frauen einen höheren Anteil an der Jagd haben, auch diesbezüglich eher ausführlichere Berichte produzieren. Bei den Kulturen, bei denen alles eh schon so ist, wie in anderen auch, also hauptsächlich die Männer jagen, wird das hingegen evtl anders sein und man wird, da nichts wirklich neues vorliegt, auch weniger ins Detail gehen.

“Reproduction” is defined as obtaining the same results as a given study using the same data and same methods, and “replication” as obtaining similar results using new data and similar methods as one or more previous studies (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2019). We did both. We first tried to reproduce Anderson et al. (2023) by following their methods to obtain the same texts from the same database. When this failed, we tracked down most of their sources and recoded them. In the interest of reproducing the study of Anderson et al. (2023) as closely as possible, we do not limit our attention to foragers. While Anderson et al. (2023) refer to all societies in their study as foragers, they did not distinguish between subsistence types in their study, labeling several horticulturalist and agriculturalist groups as foragers.
First, we explored whether Anderson et al. (2023) followed their own reported exclusion criteria. In this section of our critique, we worked with population names in the datasets as they were presented, not adjusting for potential cases of pseudoreplication. We considered, first, whether societies were included in the analysis that were not found in D-PLACE (Table S1). We identified a significant number of societies in the Anderson et al. (2023) sample that were not in the D-PLACE database (which comprises the Ethnographic Atlas, the Binford dataset, the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample (SCCS), and the Western North American Indians dataset). Specifically, of those 63 societies with “explicit data on hunting” (p.3), 22/63 (35%) were not present in D-PLACE. Of the 50/63 societies with putative evidence of women’s hunting, 18 (36%) were not found in D-PLACE. This raises the question of how societies outside of D-PLACE were chosen, as no details were given. We found that of those 22 societies gathered from outside of D-PLACE, 18/22 (81%) had evidence of women’s hunting, according to the authors. In the absence of methodological details about how bias was avoided, this result implies biased selection.

Also wenn ich es richtig verstanden habe hat man angegeben, dass man Daten aus einer bestimmten größeren Datenbank verwendet hat, es zeigt sich aber, dass gerade Völker mit Frauen als Jäger gerade nicht in dieser Datenbank waren. Der Grund könnte sein, dass man diese eben ganz bewusst heraus gesucht hat.

We next considered whether there were societies among the initial 391 D-PLACE samples for which explicit descriptions of hunting were available, but were excluded nonetheless. Here, we found evidence that Anderson et al. (2023) did not follow their own reported selection criteria and excluded some groups that should have been included, again inflating the frequency of women’s hunting. Our attempts to acquire from D-PLACE (Kirby et al., 2016) the same list of 391 societies that Anderson et al. (2023) began with did not meet with success due to a lack of sufficient information in their paper, and so we could not reproduce their analysis. Instead, to assess the possibility that some relevant ethnographies were wrongly excluded, we constrained our search to two recent large-scale and comprehensive quantitative syntheses of human foraging behavior (Koster et al., 2020; Kraft et al., 2021). We explored the overlap between these two datasets, D-PLACE, and the Anderson et al. (2023) dataset (Table S1). We found 18 societies that were described in D-PLACE, in which comprehensive explicit data on hunting existed, but which were excluded from the Anderson et al. (2023) analysis. This provides evidence that the study did not follow its own exclusion criteria. Moreover, of these 18 societies (Anbarra, Arnhem, Bari, Bororo, G/wi, Gadio Enga, Kanela, Mamainde, Maya, Mekranoti, Nukak, Onge, Piro, Shipibo, Xavante, Yanomamö, Ye’kwana, Yukpa), 9 (G/wi, Nukak, Onge, Yanomamö, Arnhem, Shipibo, Mamainde, Bari, and Maya) reported explicitly that women did not hunt, and in the other 9 only men’s hunting was mentioned. This again raises the possibility of biased selection and again serves to artificially inflate estimates of women’s hunting. A comprehensive re-analysis of the entire D-PLACE database would likely reveal more cases of improper exclusion.

Es wurden also Gesellschaften, bei denen hinreichende Informationen vorlagen, nicht in die Studie einbezogen, und die, die nicht einbezogen wurden, waren auch noch ungünstig für das gewünschte Ergebnis Frauen als Jäger darzustellen, weil dort eben die Frauen keine Jäger waren.

We next considered the inclusion of non-D-PLACE sources by Anderson et al. (2023) (Table S1). Koster et al. (2020) selected datasets that characterized hunting across the lifespan, and Kraft et al. (2021) selected studies that characterized subsistence contributions in-depth at the societal level, partly to characterize gender-based differences in foraging. As these studies summarize some of the most authoritative datasets on forager subsistence behavior, they constitute perhaps the best explicit descriptions currently available. Between these two publications, there were 60 unique societies. Of these, Anderson et al. (2023) included 18 in their study (!Kung, Ache, Agta, Aka, Baka, Batek, Bofi, Cree, Efe, Gunwinngu, Hadza, Hiwi, Inuit, Martu, Mbuti, Pume, Punan, Tsimane), and, of these, 15 were coded as positive for the occurrence of women’s hunting. 14 of these societies were contained in D-PLACE. This leaves 42 societies with comprehensive descriptions of subsistence behavior, including hunting, that they did not investigate.

Also von 60 Studien wurden 18 aufgenommen, in 15 gab es weibliche Aktivität bei der Jagd, die 42 anderen wurden ignoriert.

Of the 60 unique societies between Koster et al. (2020) and Kraft et al. (2021), 34 were contained in D-PLACE, and 26 of these societies were not (Achuara, Batek, Bofi, Dolgan, Etolo, Hiwi, Inujjuamiut, Kaul, Lufa, Machiguenga, Maku, Maring, Mvae, Mayangna, Nen, Nimboran, Nuaulu, Nunoa, Quichua, Tatuyo, Tsembega, Tsimane, Waorani, Wola, Wayana, Yassa). Given that Anderson et al. (2023) themselves appear to have sought sources outside of D-PLACE, it is not clear why many of the most comprehensively described societies were not chosen. In these populations, based on published work (Kaplan et al., 2000; Koster et al., 2020; Kraft et al., 2021), women’s hunting occurs rarely or is not reported at all. For example, Koster et al., 2020 presented data on 40 small-scale societies. The data are largely unbiased toward the description of female hunting (J. Koster, pers comm). Only 3/40 societies (Baka, Tsimane, Martu) showed evidence for women’s hunting. Similarly, the SI of Kraft et al. (2021) has detailed information on the source data, including gendered aspects of foraging.

Ich mag ja diese höfliche Form darzulegen, dass sie ihre Daten schlicht selektiv zur These dargelegt haben. „Wir sagen nicht, dass sie es gemacht haben, aber wie konnten sie diese Studien nicht einbeziehen und dazu noch gerade die weglassen, bei denen von weiblicher Beteiligung an der Jagd keine Rede ist?“

Of course, the absence of evidence does not constitute evidence of absence, and it is always possible that women’s hunting was underreported. However, given that the Koster et al. (2020) and Kraft et al. (2021) datasets drew on studies where long periods of fieldwork were conducted, in the first case with particular emphasis on hunting, it is specifically these societies where evidence of women’s hunting is least likely to have been missed. This shows that even if Anderson et al. (2023) had strictly followed their own exclusion criteria, by limiting their search to D-PLACE, they would have missed a great deal of highly pertinent evidence that runs contrary to their conclusions.

We acknowledge that this is not a precise reproduction, as Anderson et al. (2023) did not provide enough detail in their methods to make this possible. Given the challenges of working with large ethnographic databases, we sympathize with the efforts of Anderson et al. (2023) and do not believe they were intentionally cherry-picking. Our point is that selecting different societies would have resulted in a far different (lower) estimate. Moreover, adding more societies to the denominator of the frequency calculation of Anderson et al. (2023) would change their estimate drastically. This is without examining the >300 other societies that were in their original (unsourced) sample, in some of which hunting is likely described in detail. Even putting aside their numerous coding errors (see next section), Anderson et al. (2023) estimate of 79% is unreliable and probably heavily inflated. A full analysis of several hundred ethnographic reports would be necessary to arrive at a defensible estimate.

Ohrfeigen links und rechts.

Coding errors
Second, we thoroughly re-examined the source ethnographies for the 63 societies Anderson et al. (2023) claimed as providing explicit evidence of women’s hunting. Their data are included in heir Table 1 and in the associated Supporting Information data file. We describe several issues with this dataset, the coding decisions, and the inferences that were subsequently drawn.
Coding ethnographic texts on a particular topic, such as women’s hunting, is difficult because the relevant material is typically short (sometimes one sentence or less) and often ambiguous.
Coding decisions almost always require judgment calls that may differ among coders. It is therefore essential that studies of the ethnographic record identify the specific texts used for each coding decision. Because Anderson et al. (2023) did not conduct a paragraph-level analysis, we do not know what evidence was used from source ethnographies to support their coding decisions. Accordingly, we re-coded the source ethnographies used by Anderson et al. (2023). We selected text fragments that were most relevant for coding the presence of women’s hunting, the frequency of hunting, and the size of prey hunted. In our re-analysis, for a case to count as positive evidence for women’s hunting, it must involve ‘active’ participation by women (not spiritual or ritual purposes).
Although other sources contained relevant information about women’s hunting in these societies that was not consulted by Anderson et al. (2023) (see below), we confined our re-analysis to only the ethnographic sources – and sources used therein – used by Anderson et al. (2023). Thus, our goal was not to conduct an authoritative analysis of women’s hunting, nor to conduct a paragraph-level analysis of Anderson et al.’s (2023) 63 societies. Rather, our goal was to investigate how Anderson et al. (2023) applied their coding scheme to their sources. We found that they applied it unevenly and insufficiently, and in many ethnographies they cite there are explicit statements that contradict their coding. Our re-coding of the 63 societies contained in Anderson et al. (2023) can be found in Table S2, and serves as the basis for the following points.

Sie haben also bestimmte Kriterien aufgestellt und diese uneinheitlich angewendet.  Im Einzelnen:

Subsistence and population selection

The first issue pertained to subsistence and population selection. Though nominally focused on hunter-gatherer (forager) subsistence, Anderson et al. (2023) included some horticultural and large-scale agricultural societies in their dataset, incorrectly described as foragers. Mixedsubsistence horticulturalists (H, HH) and agriculturalists (A) mis-coded as foragers in their list of 63 societies include the Kikuyu (A), Wopkaimin (HH), Iroquois (A), Tsimane (H), and Matses (HH). This is likely because D-PLACE does not classify societies by subsistence type, but rather by the relative contribution of food sources to the diet. Though all anthropologists acknowledge that these historical subsistence labels are imperfect and may not reflect current subsistence behavior, it is typical in cross-cultural research to define hunter-gatherers as peoples who depend on foraging for 90% of their diet [6]. Such labels also acknowledge populations’ current land rights or lack thereof [45].

Dass wir als Menschen Agrarwirtschaft und Viehzucht betrieben haben ist evolutionär ein sehr junges Ereignis. Es sind allenfalls 20.000 Jahre, während wir eine wesentlich längere Zeit eine Jäger und Sammler Kultur waren. Gerade wenn man über Evolution des Menschen redet ist diese kurze Zeit daher von geringerer Bedeutung. Es bringt also wenig dessen Bedingungen zugrunde zu legen.

References to secondary literature
Second, we found that many references were taken from the secondary literature. Drawing on secondary sources is problematic because they are often divorced from ethnographic context and strip relevant information that might have been present in the source ethnography. Specifically, 15/63 cases were secondary references, and 14/63 cases referenced a single nearly 30-year-old secondary source [7]. Significant discrepancies are apparent between Table 1 of Anderson et  al. (2023) and their Supporting Information spreadsheet, with 31/63 cases having different references. Also, 15/63 cases on the Supporting Information table did not have a reference listed at all. Also in 15/63 cases, Anderson et al. (2023) cited a source with evidence that comes from at least one other source that they do not reference. We refer to these as undisclosed secondary sources (USS; see Table 1).

Das wäre also einfach nur nicht hinreichend gründliches arbeiten

Inconsistent coding
Third, coding was inconsistent. For example, the Jahai of Malaysia were coded as not having female hunters based on an explicit statement in van der Sluys [46] that ‘women never hunt.’ Yet this is the same type of statement found for the !Kung, which Anderson et al. (2023) coded as a
society in which women do hunt: “… women are totally excluded from hunting. Women never participate in a !Kung hunt…” [47]. Similarly, Lee (1979: 235)[48] wrote “Women do not hunt.”

Also auch da einiges an Widersprüche und wahrscheinlich die Aufnahme von Frauen als Jäger für bestimmte Stämme, bei denen das zumindest umstritten sit.

Insufficient search for source material
Fourth, though Anderson et al. (2023) investigated each society “by searching through the original references cited in D-PLACE [39], Binford [49], and by searching digitized databases and archives,” there are instances in which well-known authoritative sources were not consulted.
For example, Anderson et al. (2023) coded the Batek of Malaysia as having female hunters based on Endicott [50]. However, a more recent book by the same author provides quantitative information on female contributions. Endicott & Endicott [51] wrote: “Still, women procured 2 percent by weight of the animals hunted by nonblowpipe methods and 22 percent of all bamboo rats.” Women procured no animals using the blowpipe (Table 4.1, p. 76)[51]. The !Kung were also coded by Anderson et al (2023) as having female hunters. Yet in her famous ethnography Nisa: The Life and Words of !Kung Woman, Shostak (1982:220)[52] wrote: “!Kung women cannot be considered hunters in any serious way…” A similar case prevails for the Tsimane horticulturalists of Bolivia. The authors cite Medinaceli and Quinlan [53], but they ignore a recent case study on Tsimane women hunting [54].

Also auch hier erhebliche Unstimmigkeiten und keine Auswertungen auch relativ prominenter Stimmen zu dem Thema.

Pseudoreplication
The fifth issue concerns pseudoreplication, in which the same case is counted more than once. This leads to inflated and inaccurate estimates. There are several examples. The !Kung and Ju/hoansi are treated as independent points, but these terms refer to the same population [48] .
The same holds for the Agta and Ayta of the Philippines [9]. Moreover, the Efe, Sua, Mbuti (BaMbuti), and Bambote, and the Mardujara and Martu (Martu), are each counted independently despite being members of closely related groups [55,56]. We recognize that these errors by Anderson et al. (2023) are not deliberate. Indeed, in at least one case it may be valid to count these as independent data points. The Efe and Mbuti live nearby but are known to have divergent hunting strategies. The Efe are traditionally bow hunters, whereas the Mbuti are traditionally primarily net hunters [57]. However, due to the potential for cultural autocorrelation to inflate the frequency of women’s hunting, such decisions should be acknowledged and justified.

Ich könnte mir vorstellen, dass die erste Studie vielleicht einfach etwas war, bei dem man recht schnell eine Studie mit dem richtigen Ergebnis zusammenstellen wollte, ohne das da jetzt Spezialisten auf dem Gebiet der verschiedenen Völker vorhanden waren. Und sich als Nichtspezialist in so ein Gebiet zu stürzen ist dann immer gefährlich.

No mention of women hunting in source material

The sixth issue is that, for some references, we could not track down any mention of women’s hunting at all in the source text (e.g., Ganij). In another case, hunting was mentioned, but it did not involve women (Lardil). A further case described trapping snares being made of women’s hair, but women are not otherwise described as being involved in hunting labor (Maidu). In several instances (the Lardil, Torres Strait Islanders, Inuit, and Kalaallit) fishing was coded as hunting, a distinction that was not made clear in the manuscript text even though these categories are customarily distinguished by anthropologists [6].

Also wieder erhebliche Fehler. Teilweise war in den Texten kein Kontext zu finden, dass Frauen jagen, die Geschichte mit dem Haar klingt als hätte man entweder diesen Umstand als aktive Jagdhilfe gewertet oder aber als habe man einfach nur nach Suchbegriffen gesucht und das Vorhandensein des Wortes Frau hätte bereits gereicht. Auch das Einbeziehen des Fischefangens als Jagd dürfte vieles verfälschen, siehe oben, es ist eben keine richtige Jagd.

 

Hunting is not a binary phenomenon

The seventh issue is that Anderson et al. (2023) conflate any mention of women’s hunting with evidence of women being consistently involved in hunting, especially of large game. For example, Ruth Landes’ [58] study of Ojibwa women is cited as evidence of women hunting. Indeed, Landes describes a few cases of women who chose to be hunters. However, she also states “those women who cross the occupational line and take up men’s work, do so casually, under the pressure of circumstances or of personal inclination….[Other] Women regard them as extraordinary or queer.” That is, Landes is noting that these women are very much the exception rather than the rule (Landes 1938: 136). Likewise, for the Central Eskimo (Inuit), Boas describes two instances of women hunting, one a secondary report from which he obtained from John Rae’s 1845-46 account of his travels. Rae’s account, which Boas recounts in its entirety, is a description of women hunting sun-bathing seals opportunistically with clubs (since they don’t carry spears). The other is an account of women assisting in a communal hunting effort of breathing-hole sealing, in which the women (and children) guard breathing holes and scare the seals away, forcing them to surface at a hole where a man is stationed with his spear ready.
Again, though, Anderson et al. (2023) do not relate Boas’s comments that speak to the general pattern of behavior: For a typical day, the men go hunting, “meanwhile the women, who stay at home, are engaged in their domestic occupations, mending boots and making new clothing …
(Boas, 1889)(1964: 154)[59]. And, “the principal part of the man’s work is to provide for his family by hunting, i.e., for his wife and children….The woman has to do the household work, the sewing, and the cooking. She must look after the lamps, make and mend the tent and boat covers, prepare the skins, and bring up young dogs.” (Boas, 1889)(1964: 171-2)[59].

Auch wieder eine ziemliche Ohrfeige. Frauen, die Tiere von den nicht von Männern abgedeckten Luftlöchern wegscheuchen, bis sie bei den Männern auftauchen müssen, sind in der Hinsicht keine Jäger, schon gar nicht für die evolutionäre Betrachtung, und Frauen, die aus Not oder weil sie anders sind als die anderen Frauen jagen, sagen eben auch wenig aus. Dazu noch die ignorierten Ausführungen zu den typischen Fällen.

A re-analysis of Anderson et al.’s (2023) ethnographic sources

In light of these issues, we re-coded three of Anderson et al.’s (2023) variables using the same ethnographic sources they cite (Table 1 and Fig 1). In place of Anderson et al.’s (2023) binary hunting variable, we coded the frequency of women’s hunting in each society based on contextual information (Table 1). Our five hunting frequency categories were: no evidence for women hunting, evidence that women did not hunt, and evidence for women hunting rarely, sometimes, or frequently. We did this separately for two prey sizes. Anderson et al. (2023) coded four prey size categories: small game (1), medium game (2), large game (3), and all (4). The criteria for distinguishing small, medium, and large game were not specified or operationalized, and no standard body-size cut-off was used. In some cases, prey size was determined indirectly. For example, in some instances, prey species were coded as ‘large game’ based on being struck with large sticks and machetes. As large sticks and machetes can be used to kill prey of a range of body sizes, we find this criterion problematic. We followed a standard cut-off used in zooarchaeology and paleontology, adopting the criteria of small-medium game (<45 kg) versus large game (≥45 kg) [17,18]. Accordingly, we had two women’s hunting variables: the frequency of hunting small-medium game, and the frequency of hunting large game. We also re-coded the primary mode of subsistence for each society. See the Appendix for our detailed coding rubric and the inter-rater reliability of our independent coders

We also re-assessed the claim that a third or more of the surveyed societies showed evidence of women participating in big-game hunting by comparing the Anderson et al. (2023) prey size variable with our two hunting frequency variables. Of the 63 societies with ‘explicit evidence of
hunting’, Anderson et al. (2023) located prey sizes for 45, 15 of which they coded as hunting big game (33%), and 2 as hunting game of all sizes, for a total of 17/45 (38%) societies as women being involved in large-game hunting. In our re-coding of women’s large game hunting frequency for these 17, we instead found No evidence: 3/17 (18%); Never: 2/17 (12%); Rarely: 7/17 (41%); Sometimes: 3/17 (18%); and Frequently: 2/17 (12%). Thus, less than a third of those originally coded involved regular female large-game hunting. Of the cases that do involve largegame hunting by women, in multiple instances the ethnographer explicitly stated that it was veryrare. Some only described cases of women hunting large game alone after the death of their husbands. Other cases pertained to communal whale hunting, or involved dogs. Some occurred in the context of husband-wife pairs where women contributed to hunting success indirectly. Some cases involved firearms, a uniquely modern technology that was unavailable during human prehistory.

In our own re-coding of the frequency of big game hunting in all 63 societies (i.e., in the 17 that Anderson et al. coded as big game hunting, as well as in the 46 that they coded as no big game hunting, and ignoring pseudo-replication) we found the following, No evidence: 32/63 (51%);
Never: 6/63 (10%); Rarely: 16/63 (25%); Sometimes: 6/63 (10%); and Frequently: 3/63 (5%). Thus, in only 9/63 (14%) societies in Anderson et al. (2023) could women be said to “regularly” hunt big game (but two of those 9, the Agta and Ayta, are the same population).. See Table 1 and
Fig 3. Fig 3: Comparison of Anderson et al. (2023) prey size coding (the four panels) with our prey size re-coding (x and y axes). Dot size indicates the number of societies with that combination of women’s small and large game hunting frequency according to our coding. For comparison with
our coding, we combined the Anderson et al. small (1) and medium (2) categories. Societies inside and outside the orange boxes are those where the Anderson et al. coding of prey size is consistent and inconsistent with ours, respectively. Anderson et al. coded two societies as women
hunting both small-medium and large game, which we placed in the “Large game” panel (upper right). We categorized frequent or occasional hunting of a prey size as consistent with the claim that women hunted that prey size, and inconsistent otherwise. The selection of societies in Anderson et al. (2023) is heavily biased, so our recoded values are not representative of female hunting in foraging societies.

Due to the sampling bias and insufficient search for source material in Anderson et al. (2023), our revised estimates of the frequency of women’s hunting in their dataset are not to be taken as representative of foraging societies. Our point, rather, is that (1) the Anderson et al. (2023) data, even though likely heavily biased toward societies in which women hunt, nevertheless only support a much lower estimate of frequent hunting by women, and (2) the estimates can vary wildly depending on inclusion criteria and coding decisions.

Wieder eine Studie, die eher feministisch geleitet zu sein scheint und dessen Ergebnis stark kritisiert werden kann.

Conclusion
We agree with Anderson et al. (2023) that we should dispel the categorically false notion that forager women do not hunt or are unable to hunt, and we thank them for bringing this important topic to the public forum. Though we appreciate their intent, Anderson et al. (2023) do not overturn current consensus views on gendered divisions of labor among contemporary foragers, which are based on a substantial body of empirical evidence. Like Anderson et al (2023), we acknowledge this body of evidence is significantly filtered through the lenses and experiences of
20th-century ethnographers who may have had their own biases and interests but were likely less aware of their positionality and its impact on their subsequent ethnographic descriptions.

Wie gesagt: Ich halte die Aussage, dass nur Männer jagten und Frauen nur sammelten für einen Strohmann. Diese Studie wurde dann von vielen so verstanden als wäre weibliches jagen deutlich häufiger gewesen, ja letztendlich eher auf dem Level der Männer. Das ist auch einer der Gründe, warum ich sie hier so ausführlich bespreche. Ich werde auf diesen Artikel denke in Diskussionen noch häufiger zurückkommen.

We have outlined several conceptual and methodological concerns with Anderson et al.’s (2023) analysis. Specifically, Anderson et al.’s (2023) analysis is not reproducible because their sampling criteria are not clear and 35% of the societies in their sample do not come from DPLACE, the database they claim was the source of all the societies in their sample. Moreover, these 35% were heavily biased toward societies that they coded as ones in which women hunt.
Many other societies with extensive information on hunting are also not in D-PLACE yet were not included in their analysis, and authoritative sources on hunting in the societies in the Anderson et al. (2023) sample were not consulted. Additionally, there are at least 18 societies in D-PLACE with information on hunting that were inexplicably omitted from their analysis, none of which provide evidence for women hunters.

Finally, there were numerous coding errors. Of the 50/63 (79%) societies that Anderson et al. (2023) coded as ones in which women hunt, for example, our re-coding found that women rarely or never hunted in 16/50 (32%); we also found 2 false negatives. Overall, we found evidence in the biased Anderson et al. (2023) data set that in 35/63 (56%) societies, women hunt “Sometimes” or “Frequently”. Moreover, compared to the 17/63 (27%) societies in which women were claimed to hunt big game regularly, our re-coding found that this was true for only 9/63 (14%). A precise estimate of women’s hunting in foraging societies must await a future thorough and unbiased analysis of the ethnographic record (see, e.g., [10]), but it is certainly far less than the Anderson et al. (2023) estimate and is very unlikely to overturn the current view that it is relatively uncommon.
The fundamental issue is that women’s hunting is not a binary phenomenon, and treating it as such, especially with a very low threshold for classifying a society as one in which women hunt, obfuscates gendered divisions of labor within groups. Anthropologists have long recognized that the nature of cooperation in foragers is complex and multi-faceted, and women’s and men’s subsistence activities play important and often complementary roles. Moreover, women’s hunting has been studied for decades, and anthropologists have a good understanding of when and why it occurs. Yet, to focus on hunting at the expense of other critical activities – from gathering and food processing, to water and firewood collection, to the manufacture of clothing, shelters, and tools, to pregnancy, childbirth, nursing, childcare, and healthcare, to education, marriages, rituals, politics, and conflict resolution – is to downplay the complexity, and thereby the importance of women’s roles in the foraging lifeway. To build a more complete picture of the lives of foragers in the present and the past, it serves no one to misrepresent reality. In correcting the misapprehension that women do not hunt, we should not replace one myth with another

Es ist immer eine interessante Sache, wenn Spezialisten etwas angepisst sind, dass man auf ihrem Fachgebiet etwas schreibt, was schlicht nicht stimmt. Dann kommt genau so eine Erwiderung dabei heraus.

„Meine Freundin fragte mich, was das Unattraktivste an ihr ist“

Mal wieder ein Text, der danach schreit, dass man den Begriff des Shittests bekannter macht:

My girlfriend asked me what the most unattractive thing about her was and I responded with

„honestly, I can’t think of a thing. I’m not just saying this coz I’m tryna be nice but I can’t think of one thing. Not one. I love everything about you, there is nothing I don’t like“

She proceeded to tell me that if I truly liked her there would be something I found unattractive about her. I don’t know what to do.

The only thing I can think of is that she thinks I’m using her for her body and that if I wasn’t there would be something that I didn’t like as well but I truly am not using her for her body

Now I’m up at 12:10 at night writing this and a para about why there’s nothing I find unattractive about her. What can I do in this situation?

TL;DR My gf asked what the most unattractive thing about her was, I said nothing (honestly) and she is upset. What can I do in this situation?

Und übersetzt:

Meine Freundin fragte mich, was das Unattraktivste an ihr sei, und ich antwortete mit

„Ehrlich gesagt, mir fällt nichts ein. Ich sage das nicht nur, weil ich versuche, nett zu sein, aber mir fällt keine einzige Sache ein. Nicht eine. Ich liebe alles an dir, es gibt nichts, was ich nicht mag“.

Sie fuhr fort und sagte mir, wenn ich sie wirklich mögen würde, gäbe es etwas, das ich an ihr unattraktiv fände. Ich weiß nicht, was ich tun soll.

Das Einzige, was mir einfällt, ist, dass sie denkt, ich benutze sie wegen ihres Körpers, und dass, wenn ich das nicht täte, es auch etwas gäbe, das ich nicht mag, aber ich benutze sie wirklich nicht wegen ihres Körpers.

Jetzt bin ich um 12:10 Uhr nachts wach und schreibe dies und einen Absatz darüber, warum ich nichts an ihr unattraktiv finde. Was kann ich in dieser Situation tun?

TL;DR Meine Freundin hat mich gefragt, was das Unattraktivste an ihr ist, ich habe nichts gesagt (ehrlich gesagt) und sie ist verärgert. Was kann ich in dieser Situation tun?

Es ist ein ganz ähnlicher Shittest wie auch in der Frage „Sehe ich dick in dem Kleid aus„. Die meisten Männer werden das erst einmal intuitiv logisch durchdenken, aber tatsächlich ist eben in den meisten Fällen keine inhaltliche Erwiderung gewünscht, sondern eine auf anderen Ebenen. Ich hatte das schon einmal bei dem Artikel zum Kleid ausgeführt:

  • Selbstkundgabe: Ich bin etwas unsicher in Bezug auf meinen Körper
  • Appell: Sage mir wie schön du mich findest und nimm mir meine Unsicherheit
  • Beziehungshinweis: Du bist meine Freundin und ich liebe dich und deinen Körper

Wenn man ein Körperteil nennt, evtl sogar ein wichtiges, dann könnte es je nach Frau sein, dass man sich das noch ewig anhören muss.

Die üblichen Vorschläge zum Umgang mit Shittests wären:

  • 1. Ignorieren und Themenwechsel: Einfach über etwas anderes reden
  • 2. C&F (Cocky / Funny): Ich finde dich in keiner Weise attraktiv, ich will dich nur wegen deines Geldes!! (natürlich nur, wenn sie das als Witz erkennt)
  • 3. Reframing: In welcher Frauenzeitschrift hast du denn den Test schon wieder gelesen?
  • 4. Dominantes Verhalten: Sei nicht so unsicher, das hast du nicht nötig! (im passenden Ton). 
  • 5. Ehrlichkeit: Du könntest etwas abnehmen, wenn du es unbedingt wissen willst. (gefährlich)

Bei Reddit meint man:

She’s just trying to pick a fight. There was no winning here. You pick something how dare you? You say you love everything she found a reason to be mad.

Just typical teenage antics.

The more you feed into it the worse it’ll be. Redirect the conversation into asking her why she’s demanding a flaw from you. Ask her what prompted this and to get to the root of it.

Und:

You could tell her the most unattractive thing about her is when she tries to pick a fight with you out of nowhere, for no discernable reason. See what she does with that. I mean, she asked and got mad when you didn’t answer, right?

und:

Just want to offer the possibility that she isn’t trying to start a fight, but is just feeling insecure for whatever reason and looking for reassurance.

I’m not saying she’s definitely not trying to start a fight, but you care about this relationship, so it’s worth not jumping immediately to thinking she’s being manipulative. That her brain is trying to convince her there’s an issue because she doesn’t feel safe in this relationship at this moment. She’s reaching out for connection, so your best bet to avoid a fight is to recognise that and meet her with love.

Behaviour like that too regularly can be exhausting, but you know your girlfriend, so you should be able to say whether this is a pattern or just a one off. And if it’s a one off, just keep reassuring her. Maybe point out something trival, like ‚when you hog the duvet‘ or ‚that you like x food‘, and follow it with a ‚but I’m willing to overlook that because I love you‘ or something similar. If she keeps pushing for something more, tell her she’s just going to have to accept that she’s great.

Und:

She’s immature and is looking for Drama. Your Answer was good, if she keeps pressuring simply refuse to answer and tell her the Discussion has ended. Plenty of teenage Girls ask stupid Shit like this for no Reason.

Siehe auch:

Selbermach Mittwoch

Bitte Kommentare bündeln, wenn sie nur aus einzelnen Tweets und kurzem Kommentar bestehen und man sehr viele davon plant.

Zwischen einem Kommentar, der nur einen Link oder einen Tweet ohne Besprechung des dort gesagten enthält, sollten mindestens 5 Kommentare anderer liegen, damit noch eine Diskussion erfolgen kann.

Der Abstand der Klitoris von der Vagina sowie die Größe der Vagina könnten damit zusammenhängen, wie leicht eine Frau zum Orgasmus kommt:

Die Entfernung der Klitoris von der Vagina sowie die Größe der Vagina könnten damit zusammenhängen, wie leicht eine Frau zum Orgasmus kommt:

Das Geheimnis des weiblichen Orgasmus könnte komplexer sein, als wir alle dachten – vor allem für viele Freunde/Ehemänner da draußen. Weniger als 25 Prozent der Frauen geben an, dass sie beim Sex immer zum Orgasmus kommen, und es gibt immer noch nur wenige Hilfsmittel für Frauen, die versuchen, diesen ach so belebenden Moment zu erreichen. Eine neuere Studie, die sich mit den möglichen biologischen Ursachen des Problems befasst, kommt zu dem Ergebnis, dass Frauen, die Schwierigkeiten haben, einen Orgasmus zu erreichen, in der Regel auch eine kleinere Klitoris haben, die zudem weiter von der Vagina entfernt ist.

Obwohl die meisten Menschen mit dem besonders empfindlichen äußeren Teil der Klitoris, der so genannten Eichel, vertraut sein dürften, wissen nur wenige, dass sie sich unter der Haut in eine winkelförmige Form erstreckt. Hinter der Eichel befindet sich der Körper, der sich nach unten zum Scheideneingang hin wölbt. Von dort aus spaltet er sich in eine winkelförmige Struktur, die Crura (Plural für Crus), die sich nach außen erstreckt und hauptsächlich mit Harnröhren- und Dammgewebe sowie Nerven und Bändern verbunden ist. Ebenfalls mit der Klitoris verbunden sind zwischen den Crura zwei Schwellkörper – im Wesentlichen schwammartige Schwellkörper, die bei Stimulation anschwellen -, die die Vaginalöffnung „umarmen“, so das Museum of Sex.

Die Forscher der aktuellen Studie fanden heraus, dass Frauen, bei denen Anorgasmie diagnostiziert wurde, also die Unfähigkeit, auch nach Stimulation zum Orgasmus zu kommen, einen größeren Abstand zwischen ihrer Klitoris und der Scheidenöffnung aufwiesen. Insgesamt war der Abstand um etwa fünf bis sechs Zentimeter größer – im Durchschnitt sind es bis zu 10 Zentimeter – und diese Frauen hatten auch tendenziell eine kleinere Klitoris. „Vielleicht hat eine größere Klitoris mehr Nervenenden, und vielleicht kann die Klitoris bei direktem Kontakt und Stimulation mehr Empfindungen haben, was zu Orgasmen führt“, sagte Dr. Susan Oakley, Mitautorin der Studie und OBGYN am Good Samaritan Hospital in Cincinnati, Ohio, gegenüber LiveScience.

Die Studie aus dem Artikel:

Clitoral Size and Location in Relation to Sexual Function Using Pelvic MRI 

Abstract
Introduction
The female sexual response is dynamic; anatomic mechanisms may ease or enhance the intensity of orgasm.
Aim
The aim of this study is to evaluate the clitoral size and location with regard to female sexual function.
Methods
This cross‐sectional TriHealth Institutional Board Review approved study compared 10 sexually active women with anorgasmia to 20 orgasmic women matched by age and body mass index (BMI). Data included demographics, sexual history, serum hormone levels, Prolapse/Incontinence Sexual Questionnaire‐12 (PISQ‐12), Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI), Body Exposure during Sexual Activity Questionnaire (BESAQ), and Short Form Health Survey‐12. All subjects underwent pelvic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) without contrast; measurements of the clitoris were calculated.
Main Outcome Measures
Our primary outcomes were clitoral size and location as measured by noncontrast MRI imaging in sagittal, coronal, and axial planes.
Results
Thirty premenopausal women completed the study. The mean age was 32 years (standard deviation [SD] 7), mean BMI 25 (SD 4). The majority was white (90%) and married (61%). Total PISQ‐12 (P < 0.001) and total FSFI (P < 0.001) were higher for orgasmic subjects, indicating better sexual function. On MRI, the area of the clitoral glans in coronal view was significantly smaller for the anorgasmic group (P= 0.005). A larger distance from the clitoral glans (51 vs. 45 mm, P= 0.049) and body (29 vs. 21 mm, P= 0.008) to the vaginal lumen was found in the anorgasmic subjects. For the entire sample, larger distance between the clitoris and the vagina correlated with poorer scores on the PISQ‐12 (r = −0.44, P= 0.02), FSFI (r = −0.43, P= 0.02), and BESAQ (r = −0.37, P= 0.04).
Conclusion
Women with anorgasmia possessed a smaller clitoral glans and clitoral components farther from the vaginal lumen than women with normal orgasmic function.

Demnächst also:

Es liegt nicht an mir, deine Clitoris ist einfach zu weit weg von deiner Vagina!

Baby Reindeer und Stalking von Männern bzw Vergewaltigung von Männern

Die Serie Baby Reindeer  oder Rentierbaby thematisiert Stalking durch Frauen und sexuellen Mißbrauch von Männern:

Zum Inhalt (SPOILER!)

Bartender and comedian Donny Dunn offers a cup of tea to customer Martha to cheer her up. Martha develops a strong attachment to Donny and starts coming to the bar every day, harassing him online. She attends his shows and harasses his girlfriend, Teri, while also sending a stream of voicemails. Years earlier, Donny enters into a relationship with TV writer Darrien O’Connor, who offers him career advice, promises opportunities, and supplies him with drugs. Darrien repeatedly sexually assaults Donny during drug-induced blackouts. At some point, they cease seeing each other for years. Back in the present, Donny reports Martha to the police and she is arrested, charged, and sentenced, and she spends 9 months in prison. Darrien and Donny meet again and Darrien suggests a renewal of their collaboration. Donny reluctantly agrees.

Auf Reddit findet sich dazu auch folgende Anmerkung:

While watching the series, I didn’t realize the writer and the protagonist were the same person. I appreciated that they included a trigger warning for sexual assault (SA). Episode 4 meticulously portrayed the layers of guilt and self-blame that one experiences after an assault.

The line, „Did it all happen because I was giving off some vibe I wasn’t aware of?“ felt so realistic. The idea that predators choose victims they think they can groom and manipulate is insightful and disturbing. When that line was said, I thought it must have been written by someone with firsthand experience of SA or with help from a psychologist.

Once I learned the writer was also the person depicting these experiences, everything made much more sense. The story felt authentic and well-executed. The way it showed the aftermath of SA, including the introspection and desire to „fix“ others when you yourself are broken – the layering was amazing. Portraying such a heavy topic with that nuance and realism is impressive.

Arne berichtet darüber, dass sich aufgrund der Serie viele Männer bei Hilfsorganisationen gemeldet haben:

Eine Wohltätigkeitsorganisation, die sich gegen sexuellen Missbrauch von Männern einsetzt, verzeichnet seit der Ausstrahlung der TV-Serie „Baby Reindeer“ einen erstaunlichen Anstieg der Erstanrufer um 80 Prozent.

Die in Manchester ansässige Wohltätigkeitsorganisation „We Are Survivors“ bietet Unterstützung für Männer, einschließlich trans- und nicht-binärer Personen, die sexuellen Missbrauch, Vergewaltigung und sexuelle Ausbeutung überlebt haben.

Die Organisation teilte mit, dass sie seit der Veröffentlichung der Netflix-Hitserie – die einen Komiker dabei begleitet, wie er über vier Jahre lang von einer Frau unerbittlich belästigt und gestalkt wird und damit klar kommen muss, sexuell missbraucht worden zu sein – mit neuen Kontakten „überschwemmt“ worden ist.

In den ersten zwei Wochen nach der Ausstrahlung der Sendung gab es einen 80-prozentigen Anstieg der Anrufer, die zum ersten Mal anriefen und um Unterstützung baten.

Bemerkenswerterweise nannten 53 Prozent der Anrufer „Baby Reindeer“ als Grund für ihre Bitte um Unterstützung. Die Organisation verzeichnete auch einen 40-prozentigen Anstieg der Anrufe von jungen Menschen zwischen 26 und 35 Jahren.

Der Vorstandsvorsitzende und Gründer der Wohltätigkeitsorganisation Duncan Craig sagte, dass man zwar mit Drehbuchautoren zusammengearbeitet habe, um Geschichten von sexuellem Missbrauch von Männern in Serien wie „Eastenders“, „Hollyoaks“ und „Coronation Street“ auf den Bildschirm zu bringen, er aber noch nie eine solche Reaktion wie auf „Baby Reindeer“ erlebt habe.

„Früher haben einige Leute vielleicht Zeitungsinterviews gelesen und einen Monat später zum Telefon gegriffen“, sagte er. „Aber bei Baby Reindeer war die Reaktion absolut sofort. In den 15 Jahren, in denen ich in diesem Bereich tätig bin, habe ich so etwas noch nie erlebt.“ (…) Er fügte hinzu: „Wir hatten Leute, die die Sendung gesehen haben und von dem Medienrummel in den sozialen Medien mitgerissen wurden, so dass sie dachten: ‚Wenn alle darüber reden, kann ich das auch‘.“

Ich habe die Serie gesehen und fand sie gar nicht schlecht. Allerdings sind die Fälle eben auch sehr deutlich: Die Stalkerin ist vollkommen verrückt, aber er schafft es auch nicht von ihr loszukommen (was allerdings aufgrund ihrer Verrücktheit auch nicht  einfach war), will ihr irgendwie helfen, braucht auch ihre Aufmerksamkeit irgendwie. Auch bei dem TV Produzenten denkt man sich mitunter, warum er sich in solch gefährliche Situation begibt, mit einem ihm relativ fremden harte Drogen zu nehmen, was letztendlich damit begründet wird, dass dieser ihn lobt und ihm Talent bescheinigt und irgendwie ein Karriere in Aussicht stellt.

Habt ihr die Serie gesehen und wie seht ihr sie?

Wirken sich Prostitutionsgesetze auf die Vergewaltigungsrate aus? Beweise aus Europa

Eine interessante Studie:

European countries. Liberalizing prostitution leads to a significant decrease in rape rates, while prohibiting it leads to a significant increase. The results are stronger when rape is less severely underreported and when it is more difficult for men to obtain sex via marriage or partnership. We also provide the first evidence for the asymmetric effect of prostitution regulation on rape rates: the magnitude of prostitution prohibition is much larger than that of prostitution liberalization. Placebo tests show that prostitution laws have no impact on nonsexual crimes. Overall, our results indicate that prostitution is a substitute for sexual violence and that the recent global trend of prohibiting commercial sex (especially the Nordic model) could have the unforeseen consequence of proliferating sexual violence.

If you expel prostitutes from society, you will unsettle everything on account of lusts. (St. Augustine, in Richards 1995, p. 118)1

Quelle: Do Prostitution Laws Affect Rape Rates? Evidence from Europe (Volltext)

Aus den Daten:

Ist natürlich alles schwer zu beurteilen. Denn Vergewaltigung hat eine gewisse Dunkelziffer und auch Unterschiede in den Ländern können die Anzeigewahrscheinlichkeit beeinflussen. Die Skandinavischen Länder haben zB das nordische Modell, aber auch eine sehe hohe Anzeigenbereitschaft, wenn ich es richtig im Kopf habe.

Hier noch ein paar Grafiken:

Das scheint schon ein deutliches Ergebnis zu sein und es würde mich nicht unbedingt überraschen. Vergewaltigung hat – entgegen feministischer Thesen – eben doch einiges mit Sex zu tun und „Ausweichmöglichkeiten“ bescheren eben weniger sexuellen Frust, der bestimmte Männer dann evtl solche Taten begehen lässt.

Ein ähnliches Bild zeigt sich bei Pornos und Vergewaltigungen

Autogynephilia und Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory

Ein paar Studien aus jüngerer Zeit:

Autogynephilia: A scientific review, feminist analysis, and alternative ‘embodiment fantasies’ model

It is generally accepted within psychology and among trans health providers that transgender people who transition do so because they have a gender identity that is incongruent with their birth-assigned sex, and distinct from their sexual orientation. In contradiction to this standard model, the theory of autogynephilia posits that transgender women’s female gender identities and transitions are merely a by-product of their sexual orientations. While subsequent research has yielded numerous lines of evidence that, taken together, disprove the theory, autogynephilia is still often touted by anti-transgender groups, including trans-exclusionary feminists. Here, I provide an updated overview of the scientific case against autogynephilia. Following that, I will forward an alternative ‘embodiment fantasies’ model that explains all the available findings better than autogynephilia theory, and which is more consistent with contemporary thinking regarding gender and sexual diversity. I will also demonstrate how autogynephilia theory relies on essentialist, heteronormative, and male-centric presumptions about women and LGBTQ+ people, and as such, it is inconsistent with basic tenets of feminism.

 

How Autogynephilic Are Natal Females?

Blanchard proposed that autogynephilia is a natal male’s paraphilic sexual arousal in response to the thought or fantasy of being a woman. Furthermore, based on evidence collected from natal males with gender dysphoria, Blanchard argued that autogynephilia is the fundamental motivation among nonhomosexual males (i.e., those not exclusively attracted to men) who pursue sex reassignment surgery or live as transgender women. These ideas have been challenged by several writers who have asserted, or offered evidence, that autogynephilia is common among women. However, their evidence was weakened by problematic measures and limited comparison groups. We compared four samples of autogynephilic natal males (N = 1549), four samples of non-autogynephilic natal males (N = 1339), and two samples of natal females (N = 500), using Blanchard’s original measure: the Core Autogynephilia Scale. The autogynephilic samples had much higher mean scores compared with non-autogynephilic natal males and natal females, who were similar. Our findings refute the contention that autogynephilia is common among natal females.

 

Erotic Target Identity Inversions Among Men and Women in an Internet Sample 

Abstract
Introduction
Erotic target identity inversions (ETIIs) are poorly studied paraphilias that involve sexual arousal by the idea or fantasy of being the object of one’s sexual desires.
Aim
To conduct a large non-clinical online survey to investigate self-reported sexual arousal, behavioral expression, and psychological correlates of 4 proposed ETIIs.
Methods
A total of 736 natal males and 549 natal females responded to items about self-reported sexual arousal to the idea of acting as an animal (autoanthropomorphozoophilia) or the idea of acting as a child or infant (autonepiophilia), natal males reporting arousal to the idea of acting as a woman (autogynephilia), and natal females reporting arousal to the idea of acting as a man (autoandrophilia). Data pertaining to sexual orientation, childhood gender nonconformity, gender identity discomfort, autism, masochism, and humiliation were also collected.
Main Outcome Measures
The main outcome was a measure of self-reported arousal and expression of the ETIIs being explored using 4 items: arousal level (–3 to 3) when imagining being the erotic target exemplar; frequency of engagement in dressing or behaving like their preferred target (0–4); strength of feeling that they would be better off as the target (0–4); and the frequency of consideration of making physical changes to look or function more like the target (0–4).
Results
Mild levels of reported sexual arousal to the idea of being the preferred erotic target were common among the 4 groups, characterizing about half of them. Gender identity discomfort was associated with autogynephilia, autoandrophilia, and autoanthropomorphozoophilia. Greater gender nonconformity was associated with autogynephilia, autoandrophilia, and autonepiophilia. Autism scores were associated with autoandrophilia and autonepiophilia. Masochism was not associated with ETII scores, but humiliation was.
Clinical Implications
Findings suggest that it may be important to distinguish between subgroups of those with different levels and types of ETII arousal/expression.
Strengths & Limitations
Strengths of this study include the large, non-clinical sample of men and women for the investigation of ETIIs and the inclusion of measures of psychological correlates. The use of an Internet sample with self-report measures may be unrepresentative, although the Internet has the advantage of allowing recruitment from stigmatized or unusual groups. The cross-sectional nature limits our conclusions, as no causal inferences can be made.
Conclusion
The results support the concept of ETIIs as a paraphilic dimension in non-clinical samples and the possible role of gender-related psychological factors.

 

Elaborating and Testing Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory in Three Paraphilic Samples

 

Some men sexually attracted to types of persons (e.g., women) or things (e.g., animals) also have internalized sexual attractions: sexual arousal by the idea of being the type of person or thing to whom they are attracted. Consequently, some of these men develop erotic target identity inversions, in which they imitate, yearn to be, or identify as an instance of their erotic target. Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory predicts that for every external erotic target to which men are attracted, a subset of men will develop an internalized sexual attraction, which may cause an erotic target identity inversion. We examined these predictions in Internet surveys of three samples of men with paraphilic sexual interests: 322 men attracted to amputees, 1501 men attracted to animals, and 402 men attracted to severely obese persons. All samples included substantial minorities of men reporting internalized sexual attractions and erotic target identity inversions specific to their external sexual attractions (e.g., men attracted to amputees who are also aroused by the fantasy of being amputees and wish to become amputees). The correlation between degree of each internalized sexual attraction and degree of its corresponding erotic target identity inversion was approximately 1.0 after correction for attenuation. In each sample, participants’ specific internalized sexual attraction was positively correlated with autogynephilia, likely the most common internalized sexual attraction in men. Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory can potentially explain a variety of otherwise puzzling phenomena, including transgender identity among female-attracted natal males and men seeking amputations of healthy limbs.